As soon as spring comes back to the temperate10 zone, myriads11 and myriads of birds which are scattered12 over the warmer regions of the South come together in numberless bands, and, full of vigour13 and joy, hasten northwards to rear their offspring. Each of our hedges, each grove14, each ocean cliff, and each of the lakes and ponds with which Northern America, Northern Europe, and Northern Asia are dotted tell us at that time of the year the tale of what mutual aid means for the birds; what force, energy, and protection it confers to every living being, however feeble and defenceless it otherwise might be. Take, for instance, one of the numberless lakes of the Russian and Siberian Steppes. Its shores are peopled with myriads of aquatic15 birds, belonging to at least a score of different species, all living in perfect peace — all protecting one another.
“For several hundred yards from the shore the air is filled with gulls16 and terns, as with snow-flakes on a winter day. Thousands of plovers17 and sand-coursers run over the beach, searching their. food, whistling, and simply enjoying life. Further on, on almost each wave, a duck is rocking, while higher up you notice the flocks of the Casarki ducks. Exuberant18 life swarms20 everywhere.”1
And here are the robbers — the strongest, the most cunning ones, those “ideally organized for robbery.” And you hear their hungry, angry, dismal21 cries as for hours in succession they watch the opportunity of snatching from this mass of living beings one single unprotected individual. But as soon as they approach, their presence is signalled by dozens of voluntary sentries22, and hundreds of gulls and terns set to chase the robber. Maddened by hunger, the robber soon abandons his usual precautions: he suddenly dashes into the living mass; but, attacked from all sides, he again is compelled to retreat. From sheer despair he falls upon the wild ducks; but the intelligent, social birds rapidly gather in a flock and fly away if the robber is an erne; they plunge23 into the lake if it is a falcon24; or they raise a cloud of water-dust and bewilder the assailant if it is a kite.2 And while life continues to swarm19 on the lake, the robber flies away with cries of anger, and looks out for carrion25, or for a young bird or a field-mouse not yet used to obey in time the warnings of its comrades. In the face of an exuberant life, the ideally-armed robber must be satisfied with the off-fall of that life.
Further north, in the Arctic archipelagoes,
“you may sail along the coast for many miles and see all the ledges26, all the cliffs and corners of the mountain-sides, up to a height of from two to five hundred feet, literally27 covered with sea-birds, whose white breasts show against the dark rocks as if the rocks were closely sprinkled with chalk specks28. The air, near and far, is, so to say, full with fowls29.”3
Each of such “bird-mountains” is a living illustration of mutual aid, as well as of the infinite variety of characters, individual and specific, resulting from social life. The oyster-catcher is renowned30 for its readiness to attack the birds of prey31. The barge32 is known for its watchfulness33, and it easily becomes the leader of more placid34 birds. The turnstone, when surrounded by comrades belonging to more energetic species, is a rather timorous35 bird; but it undertakes to keep watch for the security of the commonwealth36 when surrounded by smaller birds. Here you have the dominative swans; there, the extremely sociable4 kittiwake-gulls, among whom quarrels are rare and short; the prepossessing polar guillemots, which continually caress37 each other; the egoist she-goose, who has repudiated38 the orphans39 of a killed comrade; and, by her side, another female who adopts any one’s orphans, and now paddles surrounded by fifty or sixty youngsters, whom she conducts and cares for as if they all were her own breed. Side by side with the penguins40, which steal one another’s eggs, you have the dotterels, whose family relations are so “charming and touching42” that even passionate43 hunters recoil44 from shooting a female surrounded by her young ones; or the eider-ducks, among which (like the velvet-ducks, or the coroyas of the Savannahs) several females hatch together in the same, nest. or the lums, which sit in turn upon a common covey. Nature is variety itself, offering all possible varieties of characters, from the basest to the highest: and that is why she cannot be depicted45 by any sweeping46 assertion. Still less can she be judged from the moralist’s point of view, because the views of the moralist are themselves a result — mostly unconscious — of the observation of Nature.
Coming together at nesting-time is so common with most birds that more examples are scarcely needed. Our trees are crowned with groups of crows’ nests; our hedges are full of nests of smaller birds; our farmhouses47 give shelter to colonies of swallows; our old towers are the refuge of hundreds of nocturnal birds; and pages might be filled with the most charming descriptions of the peace and harmony which prevail in almost all these nesting associations. As to the protection derived48 by the weakest birds from their unions, it is evident. That excellent observer, Dr. Coues, saw, for instance, the little cliff-swallows nesting in the immediate49 neighbourhood of the prairie falcon (Falco polyargus). The falcon had its nest on the top of one of the minarets50 of clay which are so common in the canons of Colorado, while a colony of swallows nested just beneath. The little peaceful birds had no fear of their rapacious51 neighbour; they never let it approach to their colony. They immediately surrounded it and chased it, so that it had to make off at once.4
Life in societies does not cease when the nesting period is over; it begins then in a new form. The young broods gather in societies of youngsters, generally including several species. Social life is practised at that time chiefly for its own sake — partly for security, but chiefly for the pleasures derived from it. So we see in our forests the societies formed by the young nuthatchers (Sitta caesia), together with tit-mouses, chaffinches, wrens52, tree-creepers, or some wood-peckers.5 In Spain the swallow is met with in company with kestrels, fly-catchers, and even pigeons. In the Far West of America the young horned larks54 live in large societies, together with another lark53 (Sprague’s), the skylark, the Savannah sparrow, and several species of buntings and longspurs.6 In fact, it would be much easier to describe the species which live isolated55 than to simply name those species which join the autumnal societies of young birds — not for hunting or nesting purposes, but simply to enjoy life in society and to spend their time in plays and sports, after having given a few hours every day to find their daily food.
And, finally, we have that immense display of mutual aid among birds-their migrations — which I dare not even enter upon in this place. Sufficient to say that birds which have lived for months in small bands scattered over a wide territory gather in thousands; they come together at a given place, for several days in succession, before they start, and they evidently discuss the particulars of the journey. Some species will indulge every afternoon in flights preparatory to the long passage. All wait for their tardy56 congeners, and finally they start in a certain well chosen direction — a fruit of accumulated collective experience — the strongest flying at the head of the band, and relieving one another in that difficult task. They cross the seas in large bands consisting of both big and small birds, and when they return next spring they repair to the same spot, and, in most cases, each of them takes possession of the very same nest which it had built or repaired the previous year.7
This subject is so vast, and yet so imperfectly studied; it offers so many striking illustrations of mutual-aid habits, subsidiary to the main fact of migration1 — each of which would, however, require a special study — that I must refrain from entering here into more details. I can only cursorily57 refer to the numerous and animated58 gatherings59 of birds which take place, always on the same spot, before they begin their long journeys north or south, as also those which one sees in the north, after the birds have arrived at their breeding-places on the Yenisei or in the northern counties of England. For many days in succession — sometimes one month — they will come together every morning for one hour, before flying in search of food — perhaps discussing the spot where they are going to build their nests.8 And if, during the migration, their columns are overtaken by a storm, birds of the most different species will be brought together by common misfortune. The birds which are not exactly migratory60, but slowly move northwards and southwards with the seasons, also perform these peregrinations in flocks. So far from migrating isolately, in order to secure for each separate individual the advantages of better food or shelter which are to be found in another district — they always wait for each other, and gather in flocks, before they move north or south, in accordance with the season.9
Going now over to mammals, the first thing which strikes us is the overwhelming numerical predominance of social species over those few carnivores which do not associate. The plateaus, the Alpine61 tracts62, and the Steppes of the Old and New World are stocked with herds63 of deer, antelopes65, gazelles, fallow deer, buffaloes67, wild goats and sheep, all of which are sociable animals. When the Europeans came to settle in America, they found it so densely68 peopled with buffaloes, that pioneers had to stop their advance when a column of migrating buffaloes came to cross the route they followed; the march past of the dense69 column lasting70 sometimes for two and three days. And when the Russians took possession of Siberia they found it so densely peopled with deer, antelopes, squirrels, and other sociable animals, that the very conquest of Siberia was nothing but a hunting expedition which lasted for two hundred years; while the grass plains of Eastern Africa are still covered with herds composed of zebra, the hartebeest, and other antelopes.
Not long ago the small streams of Northern America and Northern Siberia were peopled with colonies of beavers71, and up to the seventeenth century like colonies swarmed72 in Northern Russia. The flat lands of the four great continents are still covered with countless73 colonies of mice, ground-squirrels, marmots, and other rodents. In the lower latitudes74 of Asia and Africa the forests are still the abode75 of numerous families of elephants, rhinoceroses76, and numberless societies of monkeys. In the far north the reindeer78 aggregate79 in numberless herds; while still further north we find the herds of the musk-oxen and numberless bands of polar foxes. The coasts of the ocean are enlivened by flocks of seals and morses; its waters, by shoals of sociable cetaceans; and even in the depths of the great plateau of Central Asia we find herds of wild horses, wild donkeys, wild camels, and wild sheep. All these mammals live in societies and nations sometimes numbering hundreds of thousands of individuals, although now, after three centuries of gunpowder80 civilization, we find but the debris81 of the immense aggregations82 of old. How trifling83, in comparison with them, are the numbers of the carnivores! And how false, therefore, is the view of those who speak of the animal world as if nothing were to be seen in it but lions and hyenas84 plunging85 their bleeding teeth into the flesh of their victims! One might as well imagine that the whole of human life is nothing but a succession of war massacres86.
Association and mutual aid are the rule with mammals. We find social habits even among the carnivores, and we can only name the cat tribe (lions, tigers, leopards87, etc.) as a division the members of which decidedly prefer isolation88 to society, and are but seldom met with even in small groups. And yet, even among lions “this is a very common practice to hunt in company.”10 The two tribes of the civets (Viverridae) and the weasels (Mustelidae) might also be characterized by their isolated life, but it is a fact that during the last century the common weasel was more sociable than it is now; it was seen then in larger groups in Scotland and in the Unterwalden canton of Switzerland. As to the great tribe of the dogs, it is eminently89 sociable, and association for hunting purposes may be considered as eminently characteristic of its numerous species. It is well known, in fact, that wolves gather in packs for hunting, and Tschudi left an excellent description of how they draw up in a half-circle, surround a cow which is grazing on a mountain slope, and then, suddenly appearing with a loud barking, make it roll in the abyss.11 Audubon, in the thirties, also saw the Labrador wolves hunting in packs, and one pack following a man to his cabin, and killing90 the dogs. During severe winters the packs of wolves grow so numerous as to become a danger for human settlements, as was the case in France some five-and-forty years ago. In the Russian Steppes they never attack the horses otherwise than in packs; and yet they have to sustain bitter fights, during which the horses (according to Kohl’s testimony) sometimes assume offensive warfare91, and in such cases, if the wolves do not retreat promptly92, they run the risk of being surrounded by the horses and killed by their hoofs93. The prairie-wolves (Canis latrans) are known to associate in bands of from twenty to thirty individuals when they chase a buffalo66 occasionally separated from its herd64.12 Jackals, which are most courageous94 and may be considered as one of the most intelligent representatives of the dog tribe, always hunt in packs; thus united, they have no fear of the bigger carnivores.13 As to the wild dogs of Asia (the Kholzuns, or Dholes), Williamson saw their large packs attacking all larger animals save elephants and rhinoceroses, and overpowering bears and tigers. Hyenas always live in societies and hunt in packs, and the hunting organizations of the painted lycaons are highly praised by Cumming. Nay95, even foxes, which, as a rule, live isolated in our civilized96 countries, have been seen combining for hunting purposes.14 As to the polar fox, it is — or rather was in Steller’s time — one of the most sociable animals; and when one reads Steller’s description of the war that was waged by Behring’s unfortunate crew against these intelligent small animals, one does not know what to wonder at most: the extraordinary intelligence of the foxes and the mutual aid they displayed in digging out food concealed97 under cairns, or stored upon a pillar (one fox would climb on its top and throw the food to its comrades beneath), or the cruelty of man, driven to despair by the numerous packs of foxes. Even some bears live in societies where they are not disturbed by man. Thus Steller saw the black bear of Kamtchatka in numerous packs, and the polar bears are occasionally found in small groups. Even the unintelligent insectivores do not always disdain98 association.
However, it is especially with the rodents, the ungulata, and the ruminants that we find a highly developed practice of mutual aid. The squirrels are individualist to a great extent. Each of them builds its own comfortable nest, and accumulates its own provision. Their inclinations99 are towards family life, and Brehm found that a family of squirrels is never so happy as when the two broods of the same year can join together with their parents in a remote corner of a forest. And yet they maintain social relations. The inhabitants of the separate nests remain in a close intercourse100, and when the pine-cones101 become rare in the forest they inhabit, they emigrate in bands. As to the black squirrels of the Far West, they are eminently sociable. Apart from the few hours given every day to foraging102, they spend their lives in playing in numerous parties. And when they multiply too rapidly in a region, they assemble in bands, almost as numerous as those of locusts103, and move southwards, devastating104 the forests, the fields, and the gardens; while foxes, polecats, falcons105, and nocturnal birds of prey follow their thick columns and live upon the individuals remaining behind. The ground-squirrel — a closely-akin genus — is still more sociable. It is given to hoarding106, and stores up in its subterranean107 halls large amounts of edible108 roots and nuts, usually plundered110 by man in the autumn. According to some observers, it must know something of the joys of a miser111. And yet it remains112 sociable. It always lives in large villages, and Audubon, who opened some dwellings113 of the hackee in the winter, found several individuals in the same apartment; they must have stored it with common efforts.
The large tribe, of the marmots, which includes the three large genuses of Arctomys, Cynomys, and Spermophilus, is still more sociable and still more intelligent. They also prefer having each one its own dwelling114; but they live in big villages. That terrible enemy of the crops of South Russia — the souslik — of which some ten millions are exterminated115 every year by man alone, lives in numberless colonies; and while the Russian provincial116 assemblies gravely discuss the means of getting rid of this enemy of society, it enjoys life in its thousands in the most joyful117 way. Their play is so charming that no observer could refrain from paying them a tribute of praise, and from mentioning the melodious118 concerts arising from the sharp whistlings of the males and the melancholic119 whistlings of the females, before — suddenly returning to his citizen’s duties — he begins inventing the most diabolic means for the extermination of the little robbers. All kinds of rapacious birds and beasts of prey having proved powerless, the last word of science in this warfare is the inoculation120 of cholera121! The villages of the prairie-dogs in America are one of the loveliest sights. As far as the eye can embrace the prairie, it sees heaps of earth, and on each of them a prairie-dog stands, engaged in a lively conversation with its neighbours by means of short barkings. As soon as the approach of man is signalled, all plunge in a moment into their dwellings; all have disappeared as by enchantment122. But if the danger is over, the little creatures soon reappear. Whole families come out of their galleries and indulge in play. The young ones scratch one another, they worry one another, and display their gracefulness123 while standing124 upright, and in the meantime the old ones keep watch. They go visiting one another, and the beaten footpaths125 which connect all their heaps testify to the frequency of the visitations. In short, the best naturalists126 have written some of their best pages in describing the associations of the prairie-dogs of America, the marmots of the Old World, and the polar marmots of the Alpine regions. And yet, I must make, as regards the marmots, the same remark as I have made when speaking of the bees. They have maintained their fighting instincts, and these instincts reappear in captivity128. But in their big associations, in the face of free Nature, the unsociable instincts have no opportunity to develop, and the general result is peace and harmony.
Even such harsh animals as the rats, which continually fight in our cellars, are sufficiently129 intelligent not to quarrel when they plunder109 our larders130, but to aid one another in their plundering131 expeditions and migrations, and even to feed their invalids132. As to the beaver-rats or musk-rats of Canada, they are extremely sociable. Audubon could not but admire “their peaceful communities, which require only being left in peace to enjoy happiness.” Like all sociable animals, they are lively and playful, they easily combine with other species, and they have attained133 a very high degree of intellectual development. In their villages, always disposed on the shores of lakes and rivers, they take into account the changing level of water; their domeshaped houses, which are built of beaten clay interwoven with reeds, have separate corners for organic refuse, and their halls are well carpeted at winter time; they are warm, and, nevertheless, well ventilated. As to the beavers, which are endowed, as known, with a most sympathetic character, their astounding134 dams and villages, in which generations live and die without knowing of any enemies but the otter41 and man, so wonderfully illustrate136 what mutual aid can achieve for the security of the species, the development of social habits, and the evolution of intelligence, that they are familiar to all interested in animal life. Let me only remark that with the beavers, the muskrats137, and some other rodents, we already find the feature which will also be distinctive138 of human communities — that is, work in common.
I pass in silence the two large families which include the jerboa, the chinchilla, the biscacha, and the tushkan, or underground hare of South Russia, though all these small rodents might be taken as excellent illustrations of the pleasures derived by animals from social life.15 Precisely139, the pleasures; because it is extremely difficult to say what brings animals together — the needs of mutual protection, or simply the pleasure of feeling surrounded by their congeners. At any rate, our common hares, which do not gather in societies for life in common, and which are not even endowed with intense parental140 feelings, cannot live without coming together for play. Dietrich de Winckell, who is considered to be among the best acquainted with the habits of hares, describes them as passionate players, becoming so intoxicated141 by their play that a hare has been known to take an approaching fox for a playmate.16 As to the rabbit, it lives in societies, and its family life is entirely142 built upon the image of the old patriarchal family; the young ones being kept in absolute obedience143 to the father and even the grandfather.17 And here we have the example of two very closely-allied144 species which cannot bear each other — not because they live upon nearly the same food, as like cases are too often explained, but most probably because the passionate, eminently-individualist hare cannot make friends with that placid, quiet, and submissive creature, the rabbit. Their tempers are too widely different not to be an obstacle to friendship.
Life in societies is again the rule with the large family of horses, which includes the wild horses and donkeys of Asia, the zebras, the mustangs, the cimarrones of the Pampas, and the half-wild horses of Mongolia and Siberia. They all live in numerous associations made up of many studs, each of which consists of a number of mares under the leadership of a male. These numberless inhabitants of the Old and the New World, badly organized on the whole for resisting both their numerous enemies and the adverse145 conditions of climate, would soon have disappeared from the surface of the earth were it not for their sociable spirit. When a beast of prey approaches them, several studs unite at once; they repulse146 the beast and sometimes chase it: and neither the wolf nor the bear, not even the lion, can capture a horse or even a zebra as long as they are not detached from the herd. When a drought is burning the grass in the prairies, they gather in herds of sometimes 10,000 individuals strong, and migrate. And when a snow-storm rages in the Steppes, each stud keeps close together, and repairs to a protected ravine. But if confidence disappears, or the group has been seized by panic, and disperses147, the horses perish and the survivors148 are found after the storm half dying from fatigue149. union is their chief arm in the struggle for life, and man is their chief enemy. Before his increasing numbers the ancestors of our domestic horse (the Equus Przewalskii, so named by Polyakoff) have preferred to retire to the wildest and least accessible plateaus on the outskirts150 of Thibet, where they continue to live, surrounded by carnivores, under a climate as bad as that of the Arctic regions, but in a region inaccessible151 to man.18
Many striking illustrations of social life could be taken from the life of the reindeer, and especially of that large division of ruminants which might include the roebucks, the fallow deer, the antelopes, the gazelles, the ibex, and, in fact, the whole of the three numerous families of the Antelopides, the Caprides, and the Ovides. Their watchfulness over the safety of their herds against attacks of carnivores; the anxiety displayed by all individuals in a herd of chamois as long as all of them have not cleared a difficult passage over rocky cliffs. the adoption152 of orphans; the despair of the gazelle whose mate, or even comrade of the same sex, has been killed; the plays of the youngsters, and many other features, could be mentioned. But perhaps the most striking illustration of mutual support is given by the occasional migrations of fallow deer, such as I saw once on the Amur. When I crossed the high plateau and its border ridge153, the Great Khingan, on my way from Transbaikalia to Merghen, and further travelled over the high prairies on my way to the Amur, I could ascertain154 how thinly-peopled with fallow deer these mostly uninhabited regions are.19 Two years later I was travelling up the Amur, and by the end of October reached the lower end of that picturesque155 gorge156 which the Amur pierces in the Dousse-alin (Little Khingan) before it enters the lowlands where it joins the Sungari. I found the Cossacks in the villages of that gorge in the greatest excitement, because thousands and thousands of fallow deer were crossing the Amur where it is narrowest, in order to reach the lowlands. For several days in succession, upon a length of some forty miles up the river, the Cossacks were butchering the deer as they crossed the Amur, in which already floated a good deal of ice. Thousands were killed every day, and the exodus157 nevertheless continued. Like migrations were never seen either before or since, and this one must have been called for by an early and heavy snow-fall in the Great Khingan, which compelled the deer to make a desperate attempt at reaching the lowlands in the east of the Dousse mountains. Indeed, a few days later the Dousse-alin was also buried under snow two or three feet deep. Now, when one imagines the immense territory (almost as big as Great Britain) from which the scattered groups of deer must have gathered for a migration which was undertaken under the pressure of exceptional circumstances, and realizes the difficulties which had to be overcome before all the deer came to the common idea of crossing the Amur further south, where it is narrowest, one cannot but deeply admire the amount of sociability158 displayed by these intelligent animals. The fact is not the less striking if we remember that the buffaloes of North America displayed the same powers of combination. One saw them grazing in great numbers in the plains, but these numbers were made up by an infinity159 of small groups which never mixed together. And yet, when necessity arose, all groups, however scattered over an immense territory, came together and made up those immense columns, numbering hundreds of thousands of individuals, which I mentioned on a preceding page.
I also ought to say a few words at least about the “compound families” of the elephants, their mutual attachment160, their deliberate ways in posting sentries, and the feelings of sympathy developed by such a life of close mutual support.20 I might mention the sociable feelings of those disreputable creatures the wild boars, and find a word of praise for their powers of association in the case of an attack by a beast of prey.21 The hippopotamus161 and the rhinoceros77, too, would occupy a place in a work devoted162 to animal sociability. Several striking pages might be given to the sociability and mutual attachment of the seals and the walruses163; and finally, one might mention the most excellent feelings existing among the sociable cetaceans. But I have to say yet a few words about the societies of monkeys, which acquire an additional interest from their being the link which will bring us to the societies of primitive164 men.
It is hardly needful to say that those mammals, which stand at the very top of the animal world and most approach man by their structure and intelligence, are eminently sociable. evidently we must be prepared to meet with all varieties of character and habits in so great a division of the animal kingdom which includes hundreds of species. But, all things considered, it must be said that sociability, action in common, mutual protection, and a high development of those feelings which are the necessary outcome of social life, are characteristic of most monkeys and apes. From the smallest species to the biggest ones, sociability is a rule to which we know but a few exceptions. The nocturnal apes prefer isolated life; the capuchins (Cebus capucinus), the monos, and the howling monkeys live but in small families; and the orang-outans have never been seen by A.R. Wallace otherwise than either solitary165 or in very small groups of three or four individuals, while the gorillas166 seem never to join in bands. But all the remainder of the monkey tribe — the chimpanzees, the sajous, the sakis, the mandrills, the baboons168, and so on — are sociable in the highest degree. They live in great bands, and even join with other species than their own. Most of them become quite unhappy when solitary. The cries of distress169 of each one of the band immediately bring together the whole of the band, and they boldly repulse the attacks of most carnivores and birds of prey. Even eagles do not dare attack them. They plunder our fields always in bands — the old ones taking care for the safety of the commonwealth. The little tee-tees, whose childish sweet faces so much struck Humboldt, embrace and protect one another when it rains, rolling their tails over the necks of their shivering comrades. Several species display the greatest solicitude170 for their wounded, and do not abandon a wounded comrade during a retreat till they have ascertained171 that it is dead and that they are helpless to restore it to life. Thus James Forbes narrated172 in his Oriental Memoirs173 a fact of such resistance in reclaiming174 from his hunting party the dead body of a female monkey that one fully135 understands why “the witnesses of this extraordinary scene resolved never again to fire at one of the monkey race.”22 In some species several individuals will combine to overturn a stone in order to search for ants’ eggs under it. The hamadryas not only post sentries, but have been seen making a chain for the transmission of the spoil to a safe place; and their courage is well known. Brehm’s description of the regular fight which his caravan175 had to sustain before the hamadryas would let it resume its journey in the valley of the Mensa, in Abyssinia, has become classical.23 The playfulness of the tailed apes and the mutual attachment which reigns176 in the families of chimpanzees also are familiar to the general reader. And if we find among the highest apes two species, the orang-outan and the gorilla167, which are not sociable, we must remember that both — limited as they are to very small areas, the one in the heart of Africa, and the other in the two islands of Borneo and Sumatra have all the appearance of being the last remnants of formerly177 much more numerous species. The gorilla at least seems to have been sociable in olden times, if the apes mentioned in the Periplus really were gorillas.
We thus see, even from the above brief review, that life in societies is no exception in the animal world; it is the rule, the law of Nature, and it reaches its fullest development with the higher vertebrates. Those species which live solitary, or in small families only, are relatively178 few, and their numbers are limited. Nay, it appears very probable that, apart from a few exceptions, those birds and mammals which are not gregarious179 now, were living in societies before man multiplied on the earth and waged a permanent war against them, or destroyed the sources from which they formerly derived food. “On ne s’associe pas pour mourir,” was the sound remark of Espinas; and Houzeau, who knew the animal world of some parts of America when it was not yet affected180 by man, wrote to the same effect.
Association is found in the animal world at all degrees of evolution; and, according to the grand idea of Herbert Spencer, so brilliantly developed in Perrier’s Colonies Animales, colonies are at the very origin of evolution in the animal kingdom. But, in proportion as we ascend181 the scale of evolution, we see association growing more and more conscious. It loses its purely182 physical character, it ceases to be simply instinctive183, it becomes reasoned. With the higher vertebrates it is periodical, or is resorted to for the satisfaction of a given want — propagation of the species, migration, hunting, or mutual defence. It even becomes occasional, when birds associate against a robber, or mammals combine, under the pressure of exceptional circumstances, to emigrate. In this last case, it becomes a voluntary deviation184 from habitual185 moods of life. The combination sometimes appears in two or more degrees — the family first, then the group, and finally the association of groups, habitually186 scattered, but uniting in case of need, as we saw it with the bisons and other ruminants. It also takes higher forms, guaranteeing more independence to the individual without depriving it of the benefits of social life. With most rodents the individual has its own dwelling, which it can retire to when it prefers being left alone; but the dwellings are laid out in villages and cities, so as to guarantee to all inhabitants the benefits and joys of social life. And finally, in several species, such as rats, marmots, hares, etc., sociable life is maintained notwithstanding the quarrelsome or otherwise egotistic inclinations of the isolated individual. Thus it is not imposed, as is the case with ants and bees, by the very physiological187 structure of the individuals; it is cultivated for the benefits of mutual aid, or for the sake of its pleasures. And this, of course, appears with all possible gradations and with the greatest variety of individual and specific characters — the very variety of aspects taken by social life being a consequence, and for us a further proof, of its generality.24
Sociability — that is, the need of the animal of associating with its like — the love of society for society’s sake, combined with the “joy of life,” only now begins to receive due attention from the zoologists188.25 We know at the present time that all animals, beginning with the ants, going on to the birds, and ending with the highest mammals, are fond of plays, wrestling, running after each other, trying to capture each other, teasing each other, and so on. And while many plays are, so to speak, a school for the proper behaviour of the young in mature life, there are others, which, apart from their utilitarian189 purposes, are, together with dancing and singing, mere190 manifestations192 of an excess of forces —“the joy of life,” and a desire to communicate in some way or another with other individuals of the same or of other species — in short, a manifestation191 of sociability proper, which is a distinctive feature of all the animal world.26 Whether the feeling be fear, experienced at the appearance of a bird of prey, or “a fit of gladness” which bursts out when the animals are in good health and especially when young, or merely the desire of giving play to an excess of impressions and of vital power — the necessity of communicating impressions, of playing, of chattering193, or of simply feeling the proximity194 of other kindred living beings pervades195 Nature, and is, as much as any other physiological function, a distinctive feature of life and impressionability. This need takes a higher development and attains196 a more beautiful expression in mammals, especially amidst their young, and still more among the birds; but it pervades all Nature, and has been fully observed by the best naturalists, including Pierre Huber, even amongst the ants, and it is evidently the same instinct which brings together the big columns of butterflies which have been referred to already.
The habit of coming together for dancing and of decorating the places where the birds habitually perform their dances is, of course, well known from the pages that Darwin gave to this subject in The Descent of Man (ch. xiii). Visitors of the London Zoological Gardens also know the bower197 of the satin bower-bird. But this habit of dancing seems to be much more widely spread than was formerly believed, and Mr. W. Hudson gives in his master-work on La Plata the most interesting description, which must be read in the original, of complicated dances, performed by quite a number of birds: rails, jacanas, lapwings, and so on.
The habit of singing in concert, which exists in several species of birds, belongs to the same category of social instincts. It is most strikingly developed with the chakar (Chauna chavarris), to which the English have given the most unimaginative misnomer198 of “crested screamer.” These birds sometimes assemble in immense flocks, and in such cases they frequently sing all in concert. W.H. Hudson found them once in countless numbers, ranged all round a pampas lake in well-defined flocks, of about 500 birds in each flock.
“Presently,” he writes, “one flock near me began singing, and continued their powerful chant for three or four minutes; when they ceased the next flock took up the strains, and after it the next, and so on, until once more the notes of the flocks on the opposite shore came floating strong and clear across the water — then passed away, growing fainter and fainter, until once more the sound approached me travelling round to my side again.”
On another occasion the same writer saw a whole plain covered with an endless flock of chakars, not in close order, but scattered in pairs and small groups. About nine o’clock in the evening, “suddenly the entire multitude of birds covering the marsh199 for miles around burst forth200 in a tremendous evening song. . . . It was a concert well worth riding a hundred miles to hear.”27 It may be added that like all sociable animals, the chakar easily becomes tame and grows very attached to man.” They are mild-tempered birds, and very rarely quarrel”— we are told — although they are well provided with formidable weapons. Life in societies renders these weapons useless.
That life in societies is the most powerful weapon in the struggle for life, taken in its widest sense, has been illustrated201 by several examples on the foregoing pages, and could be illustrated by any amount of evidence, if further evidence were required. Life in societies enables the feeblest insects, the feeblest birds, and the feeblest mammals to resist, or to protect themselves from, the most terrible birds and beasts of prey; it permits longevity202; it enables the species to rear its progeny203 with the least waste of energy and to maintain its numbers albeit204 a very slow birth-rate; it enables the gregarious animals to migrate in search of new abodes205. Therefore, while fully admitting that force, swiftness, protective colours, cunningness, and endurance to hunger and cold, which are mentioned by Darwin and Wallace, are so many qualities making the individual, or the species, the fittest under certain circumstances, we maintain that under any circumstances sociability is the greatest advantage in the struggle for life. Those species which willingly or unwillingly206 abandon it are doomed207 to decay; while those animals which know best how to combine, have the greatest chances of survival and of further evolution, although they may be inferior to others in each of the faculties208 enumerated209 by Darwin and Wallace, save the intellectual faculty210. The highest vertebrates, and especially mankind, are the best proof of this assertion. As to the intellectual faculty, while every Darwinist will agree with Darwin that it is the most powerful arm in the struggle for life, and the most powerful factor of further evolution, he also will admit that intelligence is an eminently social faculty. Language, imitation, and accumulated experience are so many elements of growing intelligence of which the unsociable animal is deprived. Therefore we find, at the top of each class of animals, the ants, the parrots, and the monkeys, all combining the greatest sociability with the highest development of intelligence. The fittest are thus the most sociable animals, and sociability appears as the chief factor of evolution, both directly, by securing the well-being211 of the species while diminishing the waste of energy, and indirectly212, by favouring the growth of intelligence.
Moreover, it is evident that life in societies would be utterly213 impossible without a corresponding development of social feelings, and, especially, of a certain collective sense of justice growing to become a habit. If every individual were constantly abusing its personal advantages without the others interfering214 in favour of the wronged, no society — life would be possible. And feelings of justice develop, more or less, with all gregarious animals. Whatever the distance from which the swallows or the cranes come, each one returns to the nest it has built or repaired last year. If a lazy sparrow intends appropriating the nest which a comrade is building, or even steals from it a few sprays of straw, the group interferes215 against the lazy comrade; and it is evident that without such interference being the rule, no nesting associations of birds could exist. Separate groups of penguins have separate resting-places and separate fishing abodes, and do not fight for them. The droves of cattle in Australia have particular spots to which each group repairs to rest, and from which it never deviates216; and so on.28 We have any numbers of direct observations of the peace that prevails in the nesting associations of birds, the villages of the rodents, and the herds of grass-eaters; while, on the other side, we know of few sociable animals which so continually quarrel as the rats in our cellars do, or as the morses, which fight for the possession of a sunny place on the shore. Sociability thus puts a limit to physical struggle, and leaves room for the development of better moral feelings. The high development of parental love in all classes of animals, even with lions and tigers, is generally known. As to the young birds and mammals whom we continually see associating, sympathy — not love — attains a further development in their associations. Leaving aside the really touching facts of mutual attachment and compassion217 which have been recorded as regards domesticated218 animals and with animals kept in captivity, we have a number of well certified219 facts of compassion between wild animals at liberty. Max Perty and L. Buchner have given a number of such facts.29 J.C. Wood’s narrative220 of a weasel which came to pick up and to carry away an injured comrade enjoys a well-merited popularity.30 So also the observation of Captain Stansbury on his journey to Utah which is quoted by Darwin; he saw a blind pelican221 which was fed, and well fed, by other pelicans222 upon fishes which had to be brought from a distance of thirty miles.31 And when a herd of vicunas was hotly pursued by hunters, H.A. Weddell saw more than once during his journey to Bolivia and Peru, the strong males covering the retreat of the herd and lagging behind in order to protect the retreat. As to facts of compassion with wounded comrades, they are continually mentioned by all field zoologists. Such facts are quite natural. Compassion is a necessary outcome of social life. But compassion also means a considerable advance in general intelligence and sensibility. It is the first step towards the development of higher moral sentiments. It is, in its turn, a powerful factor of further evolution.
If the views developed on the preceding pages are correct, the question necessarily arises, in how far are they consistent with the theory of struggle for life as it has been developed by Darwin, Wallace, and their followers223? and I will now briefly224 answer this important question. First of all, no naturalist127 will doubt that the idea of a struggle for life carried on through organic nature is the greatest generalization225 of our century. Life is struggle; and in that struggle the fittest survive. But the answers to the questions, “By which arms is this struggle chiefly carried on?” and “Who are the fittest in the struggle?” will widely differ according to the importance given to the two different aspects of the struggle: the direct one, for food and safety among separate individuals, and the struggle which Darwin described as “metaphorical227”— the struggle, very often collective, against adverse circumstances. No one will deny that there is, within each species, a certain amount of real competition for food — at least, at certain periods. But the question is, whether competition is carried on to the extent admitted by Darwin, or even by Wallace; and whether this competition has played, in the evolution of the animal kingdom, the part assigned to it.
The idea which permeates228 Darwin’s work is certainly one of real competition going on within each animal group for food, safety, and possibility of leaving an offspring. He often speaks of regions being stocked with animal life to their full capacity, and from that overstocking he infers the necessity of competition. But when we look in his work for real proofs of that competition, we must confess that we do not find them sufficiently convincing. If we refer to the paragraph entitled “Struggle for Life most severe between Individuals and Varieties of the same Species,” we find in it none of that wealth of proofs and illustrations which we are accustomed to find in whatever Darwin wrote. The struggle between individuals of the same species is not illustrated under that heading by even one single instance: it is taken as granted; and the competition between closely-allied animal species is illustrated by but five examples, out of which one, at least (relating to the two species of thrushes), now proves to be doubtful.32 But when we look for more details in order to ascertain how far the decrease of one species was really occasioned by the increase of the other species, Darwin, with his usual fairness, tells us:
“We can dimly see why the competition should be most severe between allied forms which fill nearly the same place in nature; but probably in no case could we precisely say why one species has been victorious229 over another in the great battle of life.”
As to Wallace, who quotes the same facts under a slightly-modified heading (“Struggle for Life between closely-allied Animals and Plants often most severe”), he makes the following remark (italics are mine), which gives quite another aspect to the facts above quoted. He says:
“In some cases, no doubt, there is actual war between the two, the stronger killing the weaker. but this is by no means necessary, and there may be cases in which the weaker species, physically230, may prevail by its power of more rapid multiplication, its better withstanding vicissitudes231 of climate, or its greater cunning in escaping the attacks of common enemies.”
In such cases what is described as competition may be no competition at all. One species succumbs232, not because it is exterminated or starved out by the other species, but because it does not well accommodate itself to new conditions, which the other does. The term “struggle for life” is again used in its metaphorical sense, and may have no other. As to the real competition between individuals of the same species, which is illustrated in another place by the cattle of South America during a period of drought, its value is impaired233 by its being taken from among domesticated animals. Bisons emigrate in like circumstances in order to avoid competition. However severe the struggle between plants — and this is amply proved — we cannot but repeat Wallace’s remark to the effect that “plants live where they can,” while animals have, to a great extent, the power of choice of their abode. So that we again are asking ourselves, To what extent does competition really exist within each animal species? Upon what is the assumption based? The same remark must be made concerning the indirect argument in favour of a severe competition and struggle for life within each species, which may be derived from the “extermination of transitional varieties,” so often mentioned by Darwin. It is known that for a long time Darwin was worried by the difficulty which he saw in the absence of a long chain of intermediate forms between closely-allied species, and that he found the solution of this difficulty in the supposed extermination of the intermediate forms.33 However, an attentive234 reading of the different chapters in which Darwin and Wallace speak of this subject soon brings one to the conclusion that the word “extermination” does not mean real extermination; the same remark which Darwin made concerning his expression: “struggle for existence,” evidently applies to the word “extermination” as well. It can by no means be understood in its direct sense, but must be taken “in its metaphoric226 sense.” If we start from the supposition that a given area is stocked with animals to its fullest capacity, and that a keen competition for the sheer means of existence is consequently going on between all the inhabitants — each animal being compelled to fight against all its congeners in order to get its daily food — then the appearance of a new and successful variety would certainly mean in many cases (though not always) the appearance of individuals which are enabled to seize more than their fair share of the means of existence; and the result would be that those individuals would starve both the parental form which does not possess the new variation and the intermediate forms which do not possess it in the same degree. It may be that at the outset, Darwin understood the appearance of new varieties under this aspect; at least, the frequent use of the word “extermination” conveys such an impression. But both he and Wallace knew Nature too well not to perceive that this is by no means the only possible and necessary course of affairs.
If the physical and the biological conditions of a given area, the extension of the area occupied by a given species, and the habits of all the members of the latter remained unchanged — then the sudden appearance of a new variety might mean the starving out and the extermination of all the individuals which were not endowed in a sufficient degree with the new feature by which the new variety is characterized. But such a combination of conditions is precisely what we do not see in Nature. Each species is continually tending to enlarge its abode; migration to new abodes is the rule with the slow snail235, as with the swift bird; physical changes are continually going on in every given area; and new varieties among animals consist in an immense number of cases-perhaps in the majority — not in the growth of new weapons for snatching the food from the mouth of its congeners — food is only one out of a hundred of various conditions of existence — but, as Wallace himself shows in a charming paragraph on the “divergence of characters” (Darwinism, p. 107), in forming new habits, moving to new abodes, and taking to new sorts of food. In all such cases there will be no extermination, even no competition — the new adaptation being a relief from competition, if it ever existed; and yet there will be, after a time, an absence of intermediate links, in consequence of a mere survival of those which are best fitted for the new conditions — as surely as under the hypothesis of extermination of the parental form. It hardly need be added that if we admit, with Spencer, all the Lamarckians, and Darwin himself, the modifying influence of the surroundings upon the species, there remains still less necessity for the extermination of the intermediate forms.
The importance of migration and of the consequent isolation of groups of animals, for the origin of new varieties and ultimately of new species, which was indicated by Moritz Wagner, was fully recognized by Darwin himself. Consequent researches have only accentuated236 the importance of this factor, and they have shown how the largeness of the area occupied by a given species — which Darwin considered with full reason so important for the appearance of new varieties — can be combined with the isolation of parts of the species, in consequence of local geological changes, or of local barriers. It would be impossible to enter here into the discussion of this wide question, but a few remarks will do to illustrate the combined action of these agencies. It is known that portions of a given species will often take to a new sort of food. The squirrels, for instance, when there is a scarcity237 of cones in the larch238 forests, remove to the fir-tree forests, and this change of food has certain well-known physiological effects on the squirrels. If this change of habits does not last — if next year the cones are again plentiful239 in the dark larch woods — no new variety of squirrels will evidently arise from this cause. But if part of the wide area occupied by the squirrels begins to have its physical characters altered — in consequence of, let us say, a milder climate or desiccation, which both bring about an increase of the pine forests in proportion to the larch woods — and if some other conditions concur240 to induce the squirrels to dwell on the outskirts of the desiccating region — we shall have then a new variety, i.e. an incipient241 new species of squirrels, without there having been anything that would deserve the name of extermination among the squirrels. A larger proportion of squirrels of the new, better adapted variety would survive every year, and the intermediate links would die in the course of time, without having been starved out by Malthusian competitors. This is exactly what we see going on during the great physical changes which are accomplished242 over large areas in Central Asia, owing to the desiccation which is going on there since the glacial period.
To take another example, it has been proved by geologists243 that the present wild horse (Equus Przewalski) has slowly been evolved during the later parts of the Tertiary and the Quaternary period, but that during this succession of ages its ancestors were not confined to some given, limited area of the globe. They wandered over both the Old and New World, returning, in all probability, after a time to the pastures which they had, in the course of their migrations, formerly left.34 Consequently, if we do not find now, in Asia, all the intermediate links between the present wild horse and its Asiatic Post–Tertiary ancestors, this does not mean at all that the intermediate links have been exterminated. No such extermination has ever taken place. No exceptional mortality may even have occurred among the ancestral species: the individuals which belonged to intermediate varieties and species have died in the usual course of events — often amidst plentiful food, and their remains were buried all over the globe.
In short, if we carefully consider this matter, and, carefully re-read what Darwin himself wrote upon this subject, we see that if the word “extermination” be used at all in connection with transitional varieties, it must be used in its metaphoric sense. As to “competition,” this expression, too, is continually used by Darwin (see, for instance, the paragraph “On Extinction”) as an image, or as a way-of-speaking, rather than with the intention of conveying the idea of a real competition between two portions of the same species for the means of existence. At any rate, the absence of intermediate forms is no argument in favour of it.
In reality, the chief argument in favour of a keen competition for the means of existence continually going on within every animal species is — to use Professor Geddes’ expression — the “arithmetical argument” borrowed from Malthus.
But this argument does not prove it at all. We might as well take a number of villages in South–East Russia, the inhabitants of which enjoy plenty of food, but have no sanitary244 accommodation of any kind; and seeing that for the last eighty years the birth-rate was sixty in the thousand, while the population is now what it was eighty years ago, we might conclude that there has been a terrible competition between the inhabitants. But the truth is that from year to year the population remained stationary245, for the simple reason that one-third of the new-born died before reaching their sixth month of life; one-half died within the next four years, and out of each hundred born, only seventeen or so reached the age of twenty. The new-comers went away before having grown to be competitors. It is evident that if such is the case with men, it is still more the case with animals. In the feathered world the destruction of the eggs goes on on such a tremendous scale that eggs are the chief food of several species in the early summer; not to, say a word of the storms, the inundations which destroy nests by the million in America, and the sudden changes of weather which are fatal to the young mammals. Each storm, each inundation246, each visit of a rat to a bird’s nest, each sudden change of temperature, take away those competitors which appear so terrible in theory.
As to the facts of an extremely rapid increase of horses and cattle in America, of pigs and rabbits in New Zealand, and even of wild animals imported from Europe (where their numbers are kept down by man, not by competition), they rather seem opposed to the theory of over-population. If horses and cattle could so rapidly multiply in America, it simply proved that, however numberless the buffaloes and other ruminants were at that time in the New World, its grass-eating population was far below what the prairies could maintain. If millions of intruders have found plenty of food without starving out the former population of the prairies, we must rather conclude that the Europeans found a want of grass-eaters in America, not an excess. And we have good reasons to believe that want of animal population is the natural state of things all over the world, with but a few temporary exceptions to the rule. The actual numbers of animals in a given region are determined247, not by the highest feeding capacity of the region, but by what it is every year under the most unfavourable conditions. So that, for that reason alone, competition hardly can be a normal condition. but other causes intervene as well to cut, down the animal population below even that low standard. If we take the horses and cattle which are grazing all the winter through in the Steppes of Transbaikalia, we find them very lean and exhausted248 at the end of the winter. But they grow exhausted not because there is not enough food for all of them — the grass buried under a thin sheet of snow is everywhere in abundance — but because of the difficulty of getting it from beneath the snow, and this difficulty is the same for all horses alike. Besides, days of glazed249 frost are common in early spring, and if several such days come in succession the horses grow still more exhausted. But then comes a snow-storm, which compels the already weakened animals to remain without any food for several days, and very great numbers of them die. The losses during the spring are so severe that if the season has been more inclement250 than usual they are even not repaired by the new breeds — the more so as all horses are exhausted, and the young foals are born in a weaker condition. The numbers of horses and cattle thus always remain beneath what they otherwise might be; all the year round there is food for five or ten times as many animals, and yet their population increases extremely slowly. But as soon as the Buriate owner makes ever so small a provision of hay in the steppe, and throws it open during days of glazed frost, or heavier snow-fall, he immediately sees the increase of his herd. Almost all free grass-eating animals and many rodents in Asia and America being in very much the same conditions, we can safely say that their numbers are not kept down by competition; that at no time of the year they can struggle for food, and that if they never reach anything approaching to over-population, the cause is in the climate, not in competition.
The importance of natural checks to over-multiplication, and especially their bearing upon the competition hypothesis, seems never to have been taken into due account The checks, or rather some of them, are mentioned, but their action is seldom studied in detail. However, if we compare the action of the natural checks with that of competition, we must recognize at once that the latter sustains no comparison whatever with the other checks. Thus, Mr. Bates mentions the really astounding numbers of winged ants which are destroyed during their exodus. The dead or half-dead bodies of the formica de fuego (Myrmica saevissima) which had been blown into the river during a gale251 “were heaped in a line an inch or two in height and breadth, the line continuing without interruption for miles at the edge of the water.”35 Myriads of ants are thus destroyed amidst a nature which might support a hundred times as many ants as are actually living. Dr. Altum, a German forester, who wrote a very interesting book about animals injurious to our forests, also gives many facts showing the immense importance of natural checks. He says, that a succession of gales252 or cold and damp weather during the exodus of the pine-moth253 (Bombyx pini) destroy it to incredible amounts, and during the spring of 1871 all these moths254 disappeared at once, probably killed by a succession of cold nights.36 Many like examples relative to various insects could be quoted from various parts of Europe. Dr. Altum also mentions the bird-enemies of the pine-moth, and the immense amount of its eggs destroyed by foxes; but he adds that the parasitic255 fungi256 which periodically infest257 it are a far more terrible enemy than any bird, because they destroy the moth over very large areas at once. As to various species of mice (Mus sylvaticus, Arvicola arvalis, and A. agrestis), the same author gives a long list of their enemies, but he remarks: “However, the most terrible enemies of mice are not other animals, but such sudden changes of weather as occur almost every year.” Alternations of frost and warm weather destroy them in numberless quantities; “one single sudden change can reduce thousands of mice to the number of a few individuals.” On the other side, a warm winter, or a winter which gradually steps in, make them multiply in menacing proportions, notwithstanding every enemy; such was the case in 1876 and 1877.37 Competition, in the case of mice, thus appears a quite trifling factor when compared with weather. Other facts to the same effect are also given as regards squirrels.
As to birds, it is well known how they suffer from sudden changes of weather. Late snow-storms are as destructive of bird-life on the English moors258, as they are in Siberia; and Ch. Dixon saw the red grouse259 so pressed during some exceptionally severe winters, that they quitted the moors in numbers, “and we have then known them actually to be taken in the streets of Sheffield. Persistent260 wet,” he adds, “is almost as fatal to them.”
On the other side, the contagious261 diseases which continually visit most animal species destroy them in such numbers that the losses often cannot be repaired for many years, even with the most rapidly-multiply ing animals. Thus, some sixty years ago, the sousliks suddenly disappeared in the neighbourhood of Sarepta, in South–Eastern Russia, in consequence of some epidemics262; and for years no sousliks were seen in that neighbourhood. It took many years before they became as numerous as they formerly were.38
Like facts, all tending to reduce the importance given to competition, could be produced in numbers. Of course, it might be replied, in Darwin’s words, that nevertheless each organic being “at some period of its life, during some season of the year, during each generation or at intervals264, has to struggle for life and to suffer great destruction,” and that the fittest survive during such periods of hard struggle for life. But if the evolution of the animal world were based exclusively, or even chiefly, upon the survival of the fittest during periods of calamities265; if natural selection were limited in its action to periods of exceptional drought, or sudden changes of temperature, or inundations, retrogression would be the rule in the animal world. Those who survive a famine, or a severe epidemic263 of cholera, or small-pox, or diphtheria, such as we see them in uncivilized countries, are neither the strongest, nor the healthiest, nor the most intelligent. No progress could be based on those survivals — the less so as all survivors usually come out of the ordeal266 with an impaired health, like the Transbaikalian horses just mentioned, or the Arctic crews, or the garrison267 of a fortress268 which has been compelled to live for a few months on half rations3, and comes out of its experience with a broken health, and subsequently shows a quite abnormal mortality. All that natural selection can do in times of calamities is to spare the individuals endowed with the greatest endurance for privations of all kinds. So it does among the Siberian horses and cattle. They are enduring; they can feed upon the Polar birch in case of need; they resist cold and hunger. But no Siberian horse is capable of carrying half the weight which a European horse carries with ease; no Siberian cow gives half the amount of milk given by a Jersey269 cow, and no natives of uncivilized countries can bear a comparison with Europeans. They may better endure hunger and cold, but their physical force is very far below that of a well-fed European, and their intellectual progress is despairingly slow. “Evil cannot be productive of good,” as Tchernyshevsky wrote in a remarkable270 essay upon Darwinism.39
Happily enough, competition is not the rule either in the animal world or in mankind. It is limited among animals to exceptional periods, and natural selection finds better fields for its activity. Better conditions are created by the elimination of competition by means of mutual aid and mutual Support.40 In the great struggle for life — for the greatest possible fulness and intensity271 of life with the least waste of energy — natural selection continually seeks out the ways precisely for avoiding competition as much as possible. The ants combine in nests and nations; they pile up their stores, they rear their cattle — and thus avoid competition; and natural selection picks out of the ants’ family the species which know best how to avoid competition, with its unavoidably deleterious consequences. Most of our birds slowly move southwards as the winter comes, or gather in numberless societies and undertake long journeys — and thus avoid competition. Many rodents fall asleep when the time comes that competition should set in; while other rodents store food for the winter, and gather in large villages for obtaining the necessary protection when at work. The reindeer, when the lichens272 are dry in the interior of the continent, migrate towards the sea. Buffaloes cross an immense continent in order to find plenty of food. And the beavers, when they grow numerous on a river, divide into two parties, and go, the old ones down the river, and the young ones up the river and avoid competition. And when animals can neither fall asleep, nor migrate, nor lay in stores, nor themselves grow their food like the ants, they do what the titmouse does, and what Wallace (Darwinism, ch. v) has so charmingly described: they resort to new kinds of food — and thus, again, avoid competition.
“Don’t compete! — competition is always injurious to the species, and you have plenty of resources to avoid it!” That is the tendency of nature, not always realized in full, but always present. That is the watchword which comes to us from the bush, the forest, the river, the ocean. “Therefore combine — practise mutual aid! That is the surest means for giving to each and to all the greatest safety, the best guarantee of existence and progress, bodily, intellectual, and moral.” That is what Nature teaches us; and that is what all those animals which have attained the highest position in their respective classes have done. That is also what man — the most primitive man — has been doing; and that is why man has reached the position upon which we stand now, as we shall see in the subsequent chapters devoted to mutual aid in human societies.

点击
收听单词发音

1
migration
![]() |
|
n.迁移,移居,(鸟类等的)迁徙 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
2
migrations
![]() |
|
n.迁移,移居( migration的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
3
rations
![]() |
|
定量( ration的名词复数 ); 配给量; 正常量; 合理的量 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
4
sociable
![]() |
|
adj.好交际的,友好的,合群的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
5
rodents
![]() |
|
n.啮齿目动物( rodent的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
6
mutual
![]() |
|
adj.相互的,彼此的;共同的,共有的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
7
multiplication
![]() |
|
n.增加,增多,倍增;增殖,繁殖;乘法 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
8
extermination
![]() |
|
n.消灭,根绝 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
9
elimination
![]() |
|
n.排除,消除,消灭 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
10
temperate
![]() |
|
adj.温和的,温带的,自我克制的,不过分的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
11
myriads
![]() |
|
n.无数,极大数量( myriad的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
12
scattered
![]() |
|
adj.分散的,稀疏的;散步的;疏疏落落的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
13
vigour
![]() |
|
(=vigor)n.智力,体力,精力 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
14
grove
![]() |
|
n.林子,小树林,园林 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
15
aquatic
![]() |
|
adj.水生的,水栖的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
16
gulls
![]() |
|
n.鸥( gull的名词复数 )v.欺骗某人( gull的第三人称单数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
17
plovers
![]() |
|
n.珩,珩科鸟(如凤头麦鸡)( plover的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
18
exuberant
![]() |
|
adj.充满活力的;(植物)繁茂的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
19
swarm
![]() |
|
n.(昆虫)等一大群;vi.成群飞舞;蜂拥而入 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
20
swarms
![]() |
|
蜂群,一大群( swarm的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
21
dismal
![]() |
|
adj.阴沉的,凄凉的,令人忧郁的,差劲的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
22
sentries
![]() |
|
哨兵,步兵( sentry的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
23
plunge
![]() |
|
v.跳入,(使)投入,(使)陷入;猛冲 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
24
falcon
![]() |
|
n.隼,猎鹰 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
25
carrion
![]() |
|
n.腐肉 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
26
ledges
![]() |
|
n.(墙壁,悬崖等)突出的狭长部分( ledge的名词复数 );(平窄的)壁架;横档;(尤指)窗台 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
27
literally
![]() |
|
adv.照字面意义,逐字地;确实 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
28
specks
![]() |
|
n.眼镜;斑点,微粒,污点( speck的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
29
fowls
![]() |
|
鸟( fowl的名词复数 ); 禽肉; 既不是这; 非驴非马 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
30
renowned
![]() |
|
adj.著名的,有名望的,声誉鹊起的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
31
prey
![]() |
|
n.被掠食者,牺牲者,掠食;v.捕食,掠夺,折磨 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
32
barge
![]() |
|
n.平底载货船,驳船 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
33
watchfulness
![]() |
|
警惕,留心; 警觉(性) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
34
placid
![]() |
|
adj.安静的,平和的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
35
timorous
![]() |
|
adj.胆怯的,胆小的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
36
commonwealth
![]() |
|
n.共和国,联邦,共同体 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
37
caress
![]() |
|
vt./n.爱抚,抚摸 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
38
repudiated
![]() |
|
v.(正式地)否认( repudiate的过去式和过去分词 );拒绝接受;拒绝与…往来;拒不履行(法律义务) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
39
orphans
![]() |
|
孤儿( orphan的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
40
penguins
![]() |
|
n.企鹅( penguin的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
41
otter
![]() |
|
n.水獭 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
42
touching
![]() |
|
adj.动人的,使人感伤的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
43
passionate
![]() |
|
adj.热情的,热烈的,激昂的,易动情的,易怒的,性情暴躁的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
44
recoil
![]() |
|
vi.退却,退缩,畏缩 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
45
depicted
![]() |
|
描绘,描画( depict的过去式和过去分词 ); 描述 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
46
sweeping
![]() |
|
adj.范围广大的,一扫无遗的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
47
farmhouses
![]() |
|
n.农舍,农场的主要住房( farmhouse的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
48
derived
![]() |
|
vi.起源;由来;衍生;导出v.得到( derive的过去式和过去分词 );(从…中)得到获得;源于;(从…中)提取 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
49
immediate
![]() |
|
adj.立即的;直接的,最接近的;紧靠的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
50
minarets
![]() |
|
n.(清真寺旁由报告祈祷时刻的人使用的)光塔( minaret的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
51
rapacious
![]() |
|
adj.贪婪的,强夺的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
52
wrens
![]() |
|
n.鹪鹩( wren的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
53
lark
![]() |
|
n.云雀,百灵鸟;n.嬉戏,玩笑;vi.嬉戏 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
54
larks
![]() |
|
n.百灵科鸟(尤指云雀)( lark的名词复数 );一大早就起床;鸡鸣即起;(因太费力而不想干时说)算了v.百灵科鸟(尤指云雀)( lark的第三人称单数 );一大早就起床;鸡鸣即起;(因太费力而不想干时说)算了 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
55
isolated
![]() |
|
adj.与世隔绝的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
56
tardy
![]() |
|
adj.缓慢的,迟缓的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
57
cursorily
![]() |
|
adv.粗糙地,疏忽地,马虎地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
58
animated
![]() |
|
adj.生气勃勃的,活跃的,愉快的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
59
gatherings
![]() |
|
聚集( gathering的名词复数 ); 收集; 采集; 搜集 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
60
migratory
![]() |
|
n.候鸟,迁移 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
61
alpine
![]() |
|
adj.高山的;n.高山植物 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
62
tracts
![]() |
|
大片土地( tract的名词复数 ); 地带; (体内的)道; (尤指宣扬宗教、伦理或政治的)短文 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
63
herds
![]() |
|
兽群( herd的名词复数 ); 牧群; 人群; 群众 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
64
herd
![]() |
|
n.兽群,牧群;vt.使集中,把…赶在一起 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
65
antelopes
![]() |
|
羚羊( antelope的名词复数 ); 羚羊皮革 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
66
buffalo
![]() |
|
n.(北美)野牛;(亚洲)水牛 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
67
buffaloes
![]() |
|
n.水牛(分非洲水牛和亚洲水牛两种)( buffalo的名词复数 );(南非或北美的)野牛;威胁;恐吓 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
68
densely
![]() |
|
ad.密集地;浓厚地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
69
dense
![]() |
|
a.密集的,稠密的,浓密的;密度大的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
70
lasting
![]() |
|
adj.永久的,永恒的;vbl.持续,维持 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
71
beavers
![]() |
|
海狸( beaver的名词复数 ); 海狸皮毛; 棕灰色; 拼命工作的人 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
72
swarmed
![]() |
|
密集( swarm的过去式和过去分词 ); 云集; 成群地移动; 蜜蜂或其他飞行昆虫成群地飞来飞去 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
73
countless
![]() |
|
adj.无数的,多得不计其数的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
74
latitudes
![]() |
|
纬度 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
75
abode
![]() |
|
n.住处,住所 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
76
rhinoceroses
![]() |
|
n.钱,钞票( rhino的名词复数 );犀牛(=rhinoceros);犀牛( rhinoceros的名词复数 );脸皮和犀牛皮一样厚 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
77
rhinoceros
![]() |
|
n.犀牛 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
78
reindeer
![]() |
|
n.驯鹿 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
79
aggregate
![]() |
|
adj.总计的,集合的;n.总数;v.合计;集合 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
80
gunpowder
![]() |
|
n.火药 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
81
debris
![]() |
|
n.瓦砾堆,废墟,碎片 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
82
aggregations
![]() |
|
n.聚集( aggregation的名词复数 );集成;集结;聚集体 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
83
trifling
![]() |
|
adj.微不足道的;没什么价值的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
84
hyenas
![]() |
|
n.鬣狗( hyena的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
85
plunging
![]() |
|
adj.跳进的,突进的v.颠簸( plunge的现在分词 );暴跌;骤降;突降 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
86
massacres
![]() |
|
大屠杀( massacre的名词复数 ); 惨败 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
87
leopards
![]() |
|
n.豹( leopard的名词复数 );本性难移 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
88
isolation
![]() |
|
n.隔离,孤立,分解,分离 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
89
eminently
![]() |
|
adv.突出地;显著地;不寻常地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
90
killing
![]() |
|
n.巨额利润;突然赚大钱,发大财 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
91
warfare
![]() |
|
n.战争(状态);斗争;冲突 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
92
promptly
![]() |
|
adv.及时地,敏捷地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
93
hoofs
![]() |
|
n.(兽的)蹄,马蹄( hoof的名词复数 )v.(兽的)蹄,马蹄( hoof的第三人称单数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
94
courageous
![]() |
|
adj.勇敢的,有胆量的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
95
nay
![]() |
|
adv.不;n.反对票,投反对票者 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
96
civilized
![]() |
|
a.有教养的,文雅的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
97
concealed
![]() |
|
a.隐藏的,隐蔽的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
98
disdain
![]() |
|
n.鄙视,轻视;v.轻视,鄙视,不屑 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
99
inclinations
![]() |
|
倾向( inclination的名词复数 ); 倾斜; 爱好; 斜坡 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
100
intercourse
![]() |
|
n.性交;交流,交往,交际 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
101
cones
![]() |
|
n.(人眼)圆锥细胞;圆锥体( cone的名词复数 );球果;圆锥形东西;(盛冰淇淋的)锥形蛋卷筒 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
102
foraging
![]() |
|
v.搜寻(食物),尤指动物觅(食)( forage的现在分词 );(尤指用手)搜寻(东西) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
103
locusts
![]() |
|
n.蝗虫( locust的名词复数 );贪吃的人;破坏者;槐树 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
104
devastating
![]() |
|
adj.毁灭性的,令人震惊的,强有力的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
105
falcons
![]() |
|
n.猎鹰( falcon的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
106
hoarding
![]() |
|
n.贮藏;积蓄;临时围墙;囤积v.积蓄并储藏(某物)( hoard的现在分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
107
subterranean
![]() |
|
adj.地下的,地表下的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
108
edible
![]() |
|
n.食品,食物;adj.可食用的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
109
plunder
![]() |
|
vt.劫掠财物,掠夺;n.劫掠物,赃物;劫掠 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
110
plundered
![]() |
|
掠夺,抢劫( plunder的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
111
miser
![]() |
|
n.守财奴,吝啬鬼 (adj.miserly) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
112
remains
![]() |
|
n.剩余物,残留物;遗体,遗迹 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
113
dwellings
![]() |
|
n.住处,处所( dwelling的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
114
dwelling
![]() |
|
n.住宅,住所,寓所 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
115
exterminated
![]() |
|
v.消灭,根绝( exterminate的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
116
provincial
![]() |
|
adj.省的,地方的;n.外省人,乡下人 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
117
joyful
![]() |
|
adj.欢乐的,令人欢欣的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
118
melodious
![]() |
|
adj.旋律美妙的,调子优美的,音乐性的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
119
melancholic
![]() |
|
忧郁症患者 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
120
inoculation
![]() |
|
n.接芽;预防接种 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
121
cholera
![]() |
|
n.霍乱 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
122
enchantment
![]() |
|
n.迷惑,妖术,魅力 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
123
gracefulness
![]() |
|
参考例句: |
|
|
124
standing
![]() |
|
n.持续,地位;adj.永久的,不动的,直立的,不流动的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
125
footpaths
![]() |
|
人行小径,人行道( footpath的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
126
naturalists
![]() |
|
n.博物学家( naturalist的名词复数 );(文学艺术的)自然主义者 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
127
naturalist
![]() |
|
n.博物学家(尤指直接观察动植物者) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
128
captivity
![]() |
|
n.囚禁;被俘;束缚 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
129
sufficiently
![]() |
|
adv.足够地,充分地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
130
larders
![]() |
|
n.(家中的)食物贮藏室,食物橱( larder的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
131
plundering
![]() |
|
掠夺,抢劫( plunder的现在分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
132
invalids
![]() |
|
病人,残疾者( invalid的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
133
attained
![]() |
|
(通常经过努力)实现( attain的过去式和过去分词 ); 达到; 获得; 达到(某年龄、水平、状况) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
134
astounding
![]() |
|
adj.使人震惊的vt.使震惊,使大吃一惊astound的现在分词) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
135
fully
![]() |
|
adv.完全地,全部地,彻底地;充分地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
136
illustrate
![]() |
|
v.举例说明,阐明;图解,加插图 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
137
muskrats
![]() |
|
n.麝鼠(产于北美,毛皮珍贵)( muskrat的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
138
distinctive
![]() |
|
adj.特别的,有特色的,与众不同的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
139
precisely
![]() |
|
adv.恰好,正好,精确地,细致地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
140
parental
![]() |
|
adj.父母的;父的;母的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
141
intoxicated
![]() |
|
喝醉的,极其兴奋的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
142
entirely
![]() |
|
ad.全部地,完整地;完全地,彻底地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
143
obedience
![]() |
|
n.服从,顺从 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
144
allied
![]() |
|
adj.协约国的;同盟国的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
145
adverse
![]() |
|
adj.不利的;有害的;敌对的,不友好的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
146
repulse
![]() |
|
n.击退,拒绝;vt.逐退,击退,拒绝 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
147
disperses
![]() |
|
v.(使)分散( disperse的第三人称单数 );疏散;驱散;散布 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
148
survivors
![]() |
|
幸存者,残存者,生还者( survivor的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
149
fatigue
![]() |
|
n.疲劳,劳累 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
150
outskirts
![]() |
|
n.郊外,郊区 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
151
inaccessible
![]() |
|
adj.达不到的,难接近的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
152
adoption
![]() |
|
n.采用,采纳,通过;收养 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
153
ridge
![]() |
|
n.山脊;鼻梁;分水岭 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
154
ascertain
![]() |
|
vt.发现,确定,查明,弄清 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
155
picturesque
![]() |
|
adj.美丽如画的,(语言)生动的,绘声绘色的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
156
gorge
![]() |
|
n.咽喉,胃,暴食,山峡;v.塞饱,狼吞虎咽地吃 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
157
exodus
![]() |
|
v.大批离去,成群外出 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
158
sociability
![]() |
|
n.好交际,社交性,善于交际 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
159
infinity
![]() |
|
n.无限,无穷,大量 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
160
attachment
![]() |
|
n.附属物,附件;依恋;依附 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
161
hippopotamus
![]() |
|
n.河马 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
162
devoted
![]() |
|
adj.忠诚的,忠实的,热心的,献身于...的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
163
walruses
![]() |
|
n.海象( walrus的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
164
primitive
![]() |
|
adj.原始的;简单的;n.原(始)人,原始事物 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
165
solitary
![]() |
|
adj.孤独的,独立的,荒凉的;n.隐士 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
166
gorillas
![]() |
|
n.大猩猩( gorilla的名词复数 );暴徒,打手 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
167
gorilla
![]() |
|
n.大猩猩,暴徒,打手 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
168
baboons
![]() |
|
n.狒狒( baboon的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
169
distress
![]() |
|
n.苦恼,痛苦,不舒适;不幸;vt.使悲痛 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
170
solicitude
![]() |
|
n.焦虑 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
171
ascertained
![]() |
|
v.弄清,确定,查明( ascertain的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
172
narrated
![]() |
|
v.故事( narrate的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
173
memoirs
![]() |
|
n.回忆录;回忆录传( mem,自oir的名词复数) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
174
reclaiming
![]() |
|
v.开拓( reclaim的现在分词 );要求收回;从废料中回收(有用的材料);挽救 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
175
caravan
![]() |
|
n.大蓬车;活动房屋 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
176
reigns
![]() |
|
n.君主的统治( reign的名词复数 );君主统治时期;任期;当政期 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
177
formerly
![]() |
|
adv.从前,以前 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
178
relatively
![]() |
|
adv.比较...地,相对地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
179
gregarious
![]() |
|
adj.群居的,喜好群居的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
180
affected
![]() |
|
adj.不自然的,假装的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
181
ascend
![]() |
|
vi.渐渐上升,升高;vt.攀登,登上 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
182
purely
![]() |
|
adv.纯粹地,完全地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
183
instinctive
![]() |
|
adj.(出于)本能的;直觉的;(出于)天性的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
184
deviation
![]() |
|
n.背离,偏离;偏差,偏向;离题 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
185
habitual
![]() |
|
adj.习惯性的;通常的,惯常的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
186
habitually
![]() |
|
ad.习惯地,通常地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
187
physiological
![]() |
|
adj.生理学的,生理学上的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
188
zoologists
![]() |
|
动物学家( zoologist的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
189
utilitarian
![]() |
|
adj.实用的,功利的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
190
mere
![]() |
|
adj.纯粹的;仅仅,只不过 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
191
manifestation
![]() |
|
n.表现形式;表明;现象 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
192
manifestations
![]() |
|
n.表示,显示(manifestation的复数形式) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
193
chattering
![]() |
|
n. (机器振动发出的)咔嗒声,(鸟等)鸣,啁啾 adj. 喋喋不休的,啾啾声的 动词chatter的现在分词形式 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
194
proximity
![]() |
|
n.接近,邻近 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
195
pervades
![]() |
|
v.遍及,弥漫( pervade的第三人称单数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
196
attains
![]() |
|
(通常经过努力)实现( attain的第三人称单数 ); 达到; 获得; 达到(某年龄、水平、状况) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
197
bower
![]() |
|
n.凉亭,树荫下凉快之处;闺房;v.荫蔽 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
198
misnomer
![]() |
|
n.误称 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
199
marsh
![]() |
|
n.沼泽,湿地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
200
forth
![]() |
|
adv.向前;向外,往外 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
201
illustrated
![]() |
|
adj. 有插图的,列举的 动词illustrate的过去式和过去分词 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
202
longevity
![]() |
|
n.长命;长寿 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
203
progeny
![]() |
|
n.后代,子孙;结果 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
204
albeit
![]() |
|
conj.即使;纵使;虽然 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
205
abodes
![]() |
|
住所( abode的名词复数 ); 公寓; (在某地的)暂住; 逗留 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
206
unwillingly
![]() |
|
adv.不情愿地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
207
doomed
![]() |
|
命定的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
208
faculties
![]() |
|
n.能力( faculty的名词复数 );全体教职员;技巧;院 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
209
enumerated
![]() |
|
v.列举,枚举,数( enumerate的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
210
faculty
![]() |
|
n.才能;学院,系;(学院或系的)全体教学人员 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
211
well-being
![]() |
|
n.安康,安乐,幸福 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
212
indirectly
![]() |
|
adv.间接地,不直接了当地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
213
utterly
![]() |
|
adv.完全地,绝对地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
214
interfering
![]() |
|
adj. 妨碍的 动词interfere的现在分词 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
215
interferes
![]() |
|
vi. 妨碍,冲突,干涉 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
216
deviates
![]() |
|
v.偏离,越轨( deviate的第三人称单数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
217
compassion
![]() |
|
n.同情,怜悯 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
218
domesticated
![]() |
|
adj.喜欢家庭生活的;(指动物)被驯养了的v.驯化( domesticate的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
219
certified
![]() |
|
a.经证明合格的;具有证明文件的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
220
narrative
![]() |
|
n.叙述,故事;adj.叙事的,故事体的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
221
pelican
![]() |
|
n.鹈鹕,伽蓝鸟 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
222
pelicans
![]() |
|
n.鹈鹕( pelican的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
223
followers
![]() |
|
追随者( follower的名词复数 ); 用户; 契据的附面; 从动件 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
224
briefly
![]() |
|
adv.简单地,简短地 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
225
generalization
![]() |
|
n.普遍性,一般性,概括 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
226
metaphoric
![]() |
|
adj. 使用隐喻的;比喻的;比喻意义的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
227
metaphorical
![]() |
|
a.隐喻的,比喻的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
228
permeates
![]() |
|
弥漫( permeate的第三人称单数 ); 遍布; 渗入; 渗透 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
229
victorious
![]() |
|
adj.胜利的,得胜的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
230
physically
![]() |
|
adj.物质上,体格上,身体上,按自然规律 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
231
vicissitudes
![]() |
|
n.变迁,世事变化;变迁兴衰( vicissitude的名词复数 );盛衰兴废 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
232
succumbs
![]() |
|
不再抵抗(诱惑、疾病、攻击等)( succumb的第三人称单数 ); 屈从; 被压垮; 死 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
233
impaired
![]() |
|
adj.受损的;出毛病的;有(身体或智力)缺陷的v.损害,削弱( impair的过去式和过去分词 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
234
attentive
![]() |
|
adj.注意的,专心的;关心(别人)的,殷勤的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
235
snail
![]() |
|
n.蜗牛 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
236
accentuated
![]() |
|
v.重读( accentuate的过去式和过去分词 );使突出;使恶化;加重音符号于 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
237
scarcity
![]() |
|
n.缺乏,不足,萧条 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
238
larch
![]() |
|
n.落叶松 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
239
plentiful
![]() |
|
adj.富裕的,丰富的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
240
concur
![]() |
|
v.同意,意见一致,互助,同时发生 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
241
incipient
![]() |
|
adj.起初的,发端的,初期的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
242
accomplished
![]() |
|
adj.有才艺的;有造诣的;达到了的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
243
geologists
![]() |
|
地质学家,地质学者( geologist的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
244
sanitary
![]() |
|
adj.卫生方面的,卫生的,清洁的,卫生的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
245
stationary
![]() |
|
adj.固定的,静止不动的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
246
inundation
![]() |
|
n.the act or fact of overflowing | |
参考例句: |
|
|
247
determined
![]() |
|
adj.坚定的;有决心的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
248
exhausted
![]() |
|
adj.极其疲惫的,精疲力尽的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
249
glazed
![]() |
|
adj.光滑的,像玻璃的;上过釉的;呆滞无神的v.装玻璃( glaze的过去式);上釉于,上光;(目光)变得呆滞无神 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
250
inclement
![]() |
|
adj.严酷的,严厉的,恶劣的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
251
gale
![]() |
|
n.大风,强风,一阵闹声(尤指笑声等) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
252
gales
![]() |
|
龙猫 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
253
moth
![]() |
|
n.蛾,蛀虫 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
254
moths
![]() |
|
n.蛾( moth的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
255
parasitic
![]() |
|
adj.寄生的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
256
fungi
![]() |
|
n.真菌,霉菌 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
257
infest
![]() |
|
v.大批出没于;侵扰;寄生于 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
258
moors
![]() |
|
v.停泊,系泊(船只)( moor的第三人称单数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
259
grouse
![]() |
|
n.松鸡;v.牢骚,诉苦 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
260
persistent
![]() |
|
adj.坚持不懈的,执意的;持续的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
261
contagious
![]() |
|
adj.传染性的,有感染力的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
262
epidemics
![]() |
|
n.流行病 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
263
epidemic
![]() |
|
n.流行病;盛行;adj.流行性的,流传极广的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
264
intervals
![]() |
|
n.[军事]间隔( interval的名词复数 );间隔时间;[数学]区间;(戏剧、电影或音乐会的)幕间休息 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
265
calamities
![]() |
|
n.灾祸,灾难( calamity的名词复数 );不幸之事 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
266
ordeal
![]() |
|
n.苦难经历,(尤指对品格、耐力的)严峻考验 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
267
garrison
![]() |
|
n.卫戍部队;驻地,卫戍区;vt.派(兵)驻防 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
268
fortress
![]() |
|
n.堡垒,防御工事 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
269
jersey
![]() |
|
n.运动衫 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
270
remarkable
![]() |
|
adj.显著的,异常的,非凡的,值得注意的 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
271
intensity
![]() |
|
n.强烈,剧烈;强度;烈度 | |
参考例句: |
|
|
272
lichens
![]() |
|
n.地衣( lichen的名词复数 ) | |
参考例句: |
|
|
欢迎访问英文小说网 |